- Tips for Spending Holiday Time With Family Members Who Live with Dementia
- Tainted Cucumbers Now Linked to 100 Salmonella Cases in 23 States
- Check Your Pantry, Lay’s Classic Potato Chips Recalled Due to Milk Allergy Risk
- Norovirus Sickens Hundreds on Three Cruise Ships: CDC
- Not Just Blabber: What Baby’s First Vocalizations and Coos Can Tell Us
- What’s the Link Between Memory Problems and Sexism?
- Supreme Court to Decide on South Carolina’s Bid to Cut Funding for Planned Parenthood
- Antibiotics Do Not Increase Risks for Cognitive Decline, Dementia in Older Adults, New Data Says
- A New Way to Treat Sjögren’s Disease? Researchers Are Hopeful
- Some Abortion Pill Users Surprised By Pain, Study Says
Hungry in the Department Store? Your Spending May Rise
Worried about rising credit card bills? A new study points to one way to curb your spending: Don’t shop while hungry.
Experiments suggest that hunger may push people into buying more than they would when full, and that includes nonfood items.
“I think consumers should be aware of this — that they may spend more money online or in a store if they’re hungry while they shop,” said study lead author Alison Jing Xu, assistant professor of marketing at the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis. “If you’re hungry, think twice.”
She said that hunger seems to drive people to acquire food, of course, but that biological drive may “spill over and put consumers into the mode of wanting more stuff in general.”
“Our thoughts originate in hunger and food, but once those thoughts are in our minds they end up guiding our decisions in the nonfood domain,” Xu reasoned.
Xu, along with colleagues Norbert Schwarz and Robert Wyer Jr., published the findings this week in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
According to the researchers, it has long been known that hunger causes people to spend more time and money buying more food. Other research has even suggested that hunger boosts a man’s attraction to heavier women — the theory being that heavier women may signal greater access to food.
But can the greediness that hunger prompts in people drive a desire to consume in general, whether the item is a doughnut or a DVD?
To find out, Xu’s team conducted five experiments involving about 76 participants each.
The initial group played a word game, in which they were asked to recall and make note of flashcards depicting either actual words or gibberish. Those who reported being hungrier during the test were more likely to correctly identify real words when they were related to a hunger theme (such as “hunger,” “starve,” “appetite” and “famine”) or an acquisition theme (such as “want,” “obtain” and “gain”).
A second group was polled to gauge their desire for both food and nonfood items while entering a cafe to eat, and then again after eating. The result: all indicated a greater desire for both food and nonfood items before they ate than after.
A third group was asked what they thought of a specific office binder clip and how many they’d want for a free tryout. The researchers found that while hunger didn’t seem to affect how much anyone liked or disliked the clips, people who were hungrier generally asked for more free clips.
A fourth group took part in the same binder clip experiment, but was told not to eat for four hours before being presented with a cake. Some were told to eat cake while others were not. Those who ate asked for fewer binder clip freebies than those who had no cake at testing.
It was the fifth experiment, conducted in “real world” conditions, that seemed to cinch the deal. The researchers asked actual department store shoppers how hungry they were as the shoppers exited the store (which sold mostly nonfood items). A scan of the shoppers’ receipts showed that those who were hungrier had purchased more nonfood items — even after accounting for their general mood and how much time they had spent shopping.
While the study couldn’t prove cause-and-effect, “this is the first time it has been shown that hunger can influence the acquisition of nonfood products,” Xu said.
Edward Abramson, professor emeritus of psychology at California State University in Chico, called the study “fascinating.”
“Hunger is a complex phenomenon involving the hormones and the brain,” he noted. “So it’s not necessarily that easy to make quick sense of this finding. But it occurs to me that this might have to do with hunger’s interference with impulse control. And by that I mean that the extent to which you are hungry may divert your conscious awareness of what you should buy or shouldn’t buy, or what your budget is.
“Our normal rational thinking would normally put limits on how we acquire things,” he added, “because we either don’t have the money or maybe we don’t want to appear greedy. But with hunger, maybe some of that goes out the window.”
More information
There’s more on shopping behaviors at the University of Delaware.
Source: HealthDay
Copyright © 2024 HealthDay. All rights reserved.